The temperature of emotions

Published on Jun 3, 2021in PLOS ONE3.24
· DOI :10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0252408
Francisco Barbosa Escobar3
Estimated H-index: 3
,
Carlos Velasco42
Estimated H-index: 42
+ 2 AuthorsQian Janice Wang17
Estimated H-index: 17
(AU: Aarhus University)
Sources
Abstract
Emotions and temperature are closely related through embodied processes, and people seem to associate temperature concepts with emotions. While this relationship is often evidenced by everyday language (e.g., cold and warm feelings), what remains missing to date is a systematic study that holistically analyzes how and why people associate specific temperatures with emotions. The present research aimed to investigate the associations between temperature concepts and emotion adjectives on both explicit and implicit levels. In Experiment 1, we evaluated explicit associations between twelve pairs of emotion adjectives derived from the circumplex model of affect, and five different temperature concepts ranging from 0°C to 40°C, based on responses from 403 native speakers of four different languages (English, Spanish, Japanese, Chinese). The results of Experiment 1 revealed that, across languages, the temperatures were associated with different regions of the circumplex model. The 0°C and 10°C were associated with negative-valanced, low-arousal emotions, while 20°C was associated with positive-valanced, low-to-medium-arousal emotions. Moreover, 30°C was associated with positive-valanced, high-arousal emotions; and 40°C was associated with high-arousal and either positive- or negative-valanced emotions. In Experiment 2 (N = 102), we explored whether these temperature-emotion associations were also present at the implicit level, by conducting Implicit Association Tests (IATs) with temperature words (cold and hot) and opposing pairs of emotional adjectives for each dimension of valence (Unhappy/Dissatisfied vs. Happy/Satisfied) and arousal (Passive/Quiet vs. Active/Alert) on native English speakers. The results of Experiment 2 revealed that participants held implicit associations between the word hot and positive-valanced and high-arousal emotions. Additionally, the word cold was associated with negative-valanced and low-arousal emotions. These findings provide evidence for the existence of temperature-emotion associations at both explicit and implicit levels across languages.
📖 Papers frequently viewed together
1998
1 Author (York H. Gunther)
1975
1 Author (Norman Nathan)
References108
Newest
#1Sara R. Jaeger (Plant & Food Research)H-Index: 64
#2Christina M. Roigard (Plant & Food Research)H-Index: 12
Last. Sok L. Chheang (Plant & Food Research)H-Index: 16
view all 3 authors...
Abstract Questionnaires are popular in food-related emotion research and exist in many forms. Adding to this multiplicity, a parsimonious circumplex-inspired single-response questionnaire with 12 pairs of emotion words that span the dimensions of pleasure and arousal was recently introduced [ Jaeger et al., 2020 , 103805]. It can be used with a wide range of stimuli and different stimuli formats to generate discriminative and stable product profiles. The present research advances methodological ...
Source
#1Marco TorchianoH-Index: 1
#1Domicele Jonauskaite (UNIL: University of Lausanne)H-Index: 7
#2Ahmad Abu-Akel (UNIL: University of Lausanne)H-Index: 20
Last. Christine Mohr (UNIL: University of Lausanne)H-Index: 31
view all 36 authors...
Many of us “see red,” “feel blue,” or “turn green with envy.” Are such color-emotion associations fundamental to our shared cognitive architecture, or are they cultural creations learned through ou...
Source
#1Won Kyung Lee (Yonsei University)H-Index: 6
#2So Young Sohn (Yonsei University)H-Index: 37
Abstract Do consumers become stingy tippers in inclement weather? Prior literature suggests that such weather induces negative moods, which in turn leads to stingy tipping. In this paper, on the basis of New York City taxi tipping data merged with corresponding real-time weather data, we provide different perspectives on weather-induced mood effects that take into account ambient environments provided by indoor services under inclement weather. In addition, we show that reciprocity effects trigg...
Source
#1David Bridges (University of Nottingham)H-Index: 4
#2Alain PitiotH-Index: 25
Last. Jonathan W. Peirce (University of Nottingham)H-Index: 20
view all 4 authors...
Many researchers in the behavioral sciences depend on research software that presents stimuli, and records response times, with sub-millisecond precision. There are a large number of software packages with which to conduct these behavioral experiments and measure response times and performance of participants. Very little information is available, however, on what timing performance they achieve in practice. Here we report a wide-ranging study looking at the precision and accuracy of visual and ...
Source
#1Sara R. Jaeger (Plant & Food Research)H-Index: 64
#2Christina M. Roigard (Plant & Food Research)H-Index: 12
Last. Duncan Hedderley (Plant & Food Research)H-Index: 39
view all 6 authors...
Abstract Product-centric emotion research can deepen understanding of consumers’ product experiences and food choices. Emotion measurement has become widespread in product research, and questionnaires where consumers respond to emotion words are popular and manifold. The present research adds to this multiplicity by presenting a parsimonious single-response questionnaire that spans the dimensions of pleasure and arousal, and their combinations. These are represented in a circular layout with 12 ...
Source
#1Charles Spence (University of Oxford)H-Index: 131
: A wide variety of crossmodal correspondences, defined as the often surprising connections that people appear to experience between simple features, attributes, or dimensions of experience, either physically present or else merely imagined, in different sensory modalities, have been demonstrated in recent years. However, a number of crossmodal correspondences have also been documented between more complex (i.e., multi-component) stimuli, such as, for example, pieces of music and paintings. In t...
Source
#1Zoltán Kövecses (ELTE: Eötvös Loránd University)H-Index: 28
Source
#1Haiying Wang (Qingdao University)H-Index: 3
#2Lu Liu (Qingdao University)H-Index: 1
Abstract Emotion affects people's cognition, perception and reaction to the outside world. The psychological and physiological changes brought by different emotion states may have some impact on people's perception to thermal environment. The objective of this study is to investigate whether emotion state would have some influence on people's thermal perception and comfort. In this study, experiments were carried out. Eighteen college students were recruited in the experiments. Their emotion sta...
Source
#1Charles Spence (University of Oxford)H-Index: 131
The last few years have seen an explosive growth of research interest in the crossmodal correspondences, the sometimes surprising associations that people experience between stimuli, attributes, or perceptual dimensions, such as between auditory pitch and visual size, or elevation. To date, the majority of this research has tended to focus on audiovisual correspondences. However, a variety of crossmodal correspondences have also been demonstrated with tactile stimuli, involving everything from f...
Source
Cited By1
Newest
Just as for any other sensory system, researchers have long wanted to discriminate between the sensory discriminative and hedonic aspects of tactile perception. Supporting such a distinction, researchers have, in recent decades, uncovered the existence of a dedicated system of receptors in the hairy skin (C-Tactile, CT, afferents) that appear to be preferentially tuned to pleasant stroking (i.e. caressing) touch. No matter what kind of touch one is talking about, though, it is important to recog...
Source
This website uses cookies.
We use cookies to improve your online experience. By continuing to use our website we assume you agree to the placement of these cookies.
To learn more, you can find in our Privacy Policy.