Interpersonal chemistry through negativity: Bonding by sharing negative attitudes about others

Published on Jun 1, 2006in Personal Relationships
· DOI :10.1111/J.1475-6811.2006.00109.X
Jennifer K. Bosson35
Estimated H-index: 35
(University of Texas at Austin),
Amber Johnson5
Estimated H-index: 5
(University of Texas at Austin)
+ 1 AuthorsWilliam B. Swann75
Estimated H-index: 75
(University of Texas at Austin)
Sources
Abstract
We propose that sharing a negative—as compared to a positive—attitude about a third party is particularly effective in promoting closeness between people. Findings from two survey studies and an experiment support this idea. In Studies 1 and 2, participants’ open-ended responses revealed a tendency to recall sharing with their closest friends more negative than positive attitudes about other people. Study 3 established that discovering a shared negative attitude about a target person predicted liking for a stranger more strongly than discovering a shared positive attitude (but only when attitudes were weak). Presumably, sharing negative attitudes is alluring because it establishes in-group/out-group boundaries, boosts self-esteem, and conveys highly diagnostic information about attitude holders. Despite the apparent ubiquity of this effect, participants seemed unaware of it. Instead, they asserted that sharing positive attitudes about others would be particularly effective in promoting closeness.
Figures & Tables
Download
📖 Papers frequently viewed together
569 Citations
305 Citations
326 Citations
References48
Newest
#1Catrin FinkenauerH-Index: 51
#2Peter KerkhofH-Index: 21
273 Citations
#1Roy F. Baumeister (FSU: Florida State University)H-Index: 173
#2Liqing Zhang (Case Western Reserve University)H-Index: 7
Last. Kathleen D. Vohs (UofU: University of Utah)H-Index: 91
view all 3 authors...
To complement views of gossip as essentially a means of gaining information about individuals, cementing social bonds, and engaging in indirect aggression, the authors propose that gossip serves to help people learn about how to live in their cultural society. Gossip anecdotes communicate rules in narrative form, such as by describing how someone else came to grief by violating social norms. Gossip is thus an extension of observational learning, allowing one to learn from the triumphs and misadv...
326 CitationsSource
#1Robin I. M. Dunbar (Social Science Research Council)H-Index: 125
Conversation is a uniquely human phenomenon. Analyses of freely forming conversations indicate that approximately two thirds of conversation time is devoted to social topics, most of which can be given the generic label gossip. This article first explores the origins of gossip as a mechanism for bonding social groups, tracing these origins back to social grooming among primates. It then asks why social gossip in this sense should form so important a component of human interaction and presents ev...
569 CitationsSource
#1Eric K. Foster (UPenn: University of Pennsylvania)H-Index: 1
A half century of gossip research from multiple disciplines is reviewed. Discussed are definitions of the construct; social, evolutionary, and personal functions of the practice; and data collection methods. Though people engage in the practice frequently, there has been relatively little psychological research on gossip. The layperson’s understanding of the term is included in, but insufficient to encompass, definitions used by researchers. Most data are ethnographic and discursive, and few par...
305 CitationsSource
#1Sarah R. Wert (Yale University)H-Index: 2
#2Peter Salovey (Yale University)H-Index: 131
The central thesis of this article is that all gossip involves social comparison. Research on social comparison is applied toward understanding motivations for gossip. In addition, the authors address why gossip tends to be negative and make predictions about factors that trigger especially negative talk about others. Factors such as need for moral information, powerlessness, formation and maintenance of in-groups and out-groups, and situations that bring on perceptions of injustice or feelings ...
188 CitationsSource
#1Ap Dijksterhuis (UvA: University of Amsterdam)H-Index: 65
#2Henk AartsH-Index: 72
On the basis of a functional perspective, we hypothesized that negative stimuli are detected faster than positive stimuli. In Ex- periment 1, participants were subliminally presented with positive and negative words or with no words at all. After each presentation, par- ticipants were asked whether they had seen a word. They detected neg- ative words more accurately than positive words. In Experiment 2, participants were subliminally presented with negative or positive words. After each presenta...
268 CitationsSource
The results of a field investigation indicate that people who are highly invested in their self-views (confidently held or personally important) are especially inclined to display a preference for verification of their self-views. Specifically, only those participants who were certain of their self-views or perceived them as important preferred roommates who confirmed their self-views. Such preferences were some-what stronger when the self-views were relatively negative. This is the first demons...
151 CitationsSource
#1Roy F. Baumeister (Case Western Reserve University)H-Index: 173
#2Ellen Bratslavsky (OSU: Ohio State University)H-Index: 6
Last. Kathleen D. Vohs (Case Western Reserve University)H-Index: 91
view all 4 authors...
The greater power of bad events over good ones is found in everyday events, major life events (e.g., trauma), close relationship outcomes, social network patterns, interpersonal interactions, and learning processes. Bad emotions, bad parents, and bad feedback have more impact than good ones, and bad information is processed more thoroughly than good. The self is more motivated to avoid bad self-definitions than to pursue good ones. Bad impressions and bad stereotypes are quicker to form and more...
4,612 CitationsSource
#1Henri TajfelH-Index: 40
#2John C. TurnerH-Index: 67
8,854 Citations
#1Ralph L. Rosnow (TU: Temple University)H-Index: 46
87 CitationsSource
Cited By92
Newest
#1Marta Andersson (Stockholm University)H-Index: 1
Abstract This paper investigates impoliteness and value homophily (‘thinking alike’) in the context of YouTube-based ideological discussions beneath the videos critical towards the Swedish environmental activist – Greta Thunberg. Drawing on the idea of rapport management , the study finds a remarkable scale of homophily as the postings follow recurrent patterns of face and sociality rights attacks echoing the same point of view. Consequently, while impoliteness has been recognized as widespread ...
Source
#1Mark E. Parry (UMKC: University of Missouri–Kansas City)H-Index: 31
#2Xiaoming Yang (UNO: University of Nebraska Omaha)H-Index: 6
Last. Masaaki Takemura (Meiji University)H-Index: 1
view all 3 authors...
The authors examine the role of three social motivations (social bonding, social comparison, and the sharing of social norm information) in generating word-of-mouth (WOM) among consumers in a cultu...
1 CitationsSource
#1Terence D. Dores Cruz (VU: VU University Amsterdam)H-Index: 3
#2Annika S. Nieper (VU: VU University Amsterdam)
Last. Bianca Beersma (VU: VU University Amsterdam)H-Index: 25
view all 5 authors...
The omnipresence of workplace gossip makes understanding gossip processes imperative to understand social life in organizations. Although gossip research has recently increased across the social sc...
10 CitationsSource
#1Elena Martinescu (VU: VU University Amsterdam)H-Index: 2
#2Wiebren S. Jansen (UU: Utrecht University)H-Index: 6
Last. Bianca Beersma (VU: VU University Amsterdam)H-Index: 25
view all 3 authors...
Ample experimental evidence shows that negative gossip fosters cooperation in groups by increasing individuals’ reputational concerns. However, recent field studies showed that negative gossip decr...
1 CitationsSource
#1Karen D. RudolphH-Index: 56
Source
Gossip on the work floor has only received scholarly attention in recent decades. One of the main findings of organizational gossip research is that it is ambidextrous in nature. Its double-sidedness, namely that gossip is used to blame others but also to praise others, has always been ignored, probably due to the historical negative connotations surrounding the practice of “evil tongues.” This chapter analyzes an organization’s carnival gazette full of mocking gossip leading to the conclusion t...
Source
#1Jack Corbett (University of Southampton)H-Index: 14
#2Dennis Grube (University of Cambridge)H-Index: 8
Last. Rodney J. Scott (UNSW: University of New South Wales)H-Index: 98
view all 4 authors...
How do bureaucracies remember? The conventional view is that institutional memory is static and singular, the sum of recorded files and learned procedures. There is a growing body of scholarship that suggests contemporary bureaucracies are failing at this core task. This Element argues that this diagnosis misses that memories are essentially dynamic stories. They reside with people and are thus dispersed across the array of actors that make up the differentiated polity. Drawing on four policy ex...
#1Yun Bai (Xi'an Jiaotong University)H-Index: 2
#2Jie Wang (UNNC: The University of Nottingham Ningbo China)H-Index: 8
Last. Fuli Li (Xi'an Jiaotong University)H-Index: 9
view all 4 authors...
Workplace gossip is generally viewed as a deviant behavior that negatively affects the work outcomes of employees. However, we argue that this negative view is incomplete. Drawing on the cultural l...
3 CitationsSource
#1Wouter Wolf (MPG: Max Planck Society)H-Index: 3
#2Michael Tomasello (MPG: Max Planck Society)H-Index: 184
Abstract To create social closeness, humans engage in a variety of social activities centered around shared experiences. Even simply watching the same video side by side creates social closeness in adults and children. However, perhaps surprisingly, a similar psychological mechanism was recently shown in great apes. Here we asked whether the process by which this social closeness is created is the same for children and great apes. Each participant entered a room to see an experimenter (E1) watch...
1 CitationsSource
#1Edward P. LemayH-Index: 24
#2Joshua E. Ryan (UMD: University of Maryland, College Park)H-Index: 1
Last. Michele J. Gelfand (UMD: University of Maryland, College Park)H-Index: 75
view all 4 authors...
: The current research included 7 studies testing a model of interpersonal processes when people disclose to their close relationship partners ("confidants") about their conflicts involving adversaries outside the dyad. The model posits that confidants who feel close to disclosers tend to adopt goals to be responsive to disclosers during these interactions, which motivates them to validate disclosers' negativity toward their adversaries. Disclosers interpret this validation of negativity as resp...
Source