The Mind in its Own Place: The Difficulties and Benefits of Thinking for Pleasure

Published on Jul 9, 2018
· DOI :10.1016/BS.AESP.2019.05.001
Timothy D. Wilson78
Estimated H-index: 78
(UVA: University of Virginia),
Erin C. Westgate12
Estimated H-index: 12
(UVA: University of Virginia)
+ 1 AuthorsDaniel T. Gilbert65
Estimated H-index: 65
(Harvard University)
Sources
Abstract
Abstract This chapter is concerned with a type of thinking that has received little attention, namely intentional “thinking for pleasure”—the case in which people deliberately focus solely on their thoughts with the goal of generating positive affect. We present a model that describes why it is difficult to enjoy one's thoughts, how it can be done successfully, and when there is value in doing so. We review 36 studies we have conducted on this topic with just over 10,000 participants. We found that thinking for pleasure does not come easily to most people, but can be enjoyable and beneficial under the right conditions. Specifically, we found evidence that thinking for pleasure requires both motivation and the ability to concentrate. For example, several studies show that people enjoy thinking more when it is made easier with the use of “thinking aids.” We present evidence for a trade-off model that holds that people are most likely to enjoy their thoughts if they find those thoughts to be personally meaningful, but that such thinking involves concentration, which lowers enjoyment. Lastly, we review evidence for the benefits of thinking for pleasure, including an intervention study in which participants found thinking for pleasure enjoyable and meaningful in their everyday lives.
📖 Papers frequently viewed together
34 Citations
7 Citations
2006
1 Author (Azizi Yahaya)
References120
Newest
#1Nick Buttrick (UVA: University of Virginia)H-Index: 8
#2Hyewon Choi (UVA: University of Virginia)H-Index: 7
Last. Daniela C. Wilks (University of Porto)H-Index: 6
view all 30 authors...
: Which is more enjoyable: trying to think enjoyable thoughts or doing everyday solitary activities? Wilson et al. (2014) found that American participants much preferred solitary everyday activities, such as reading or watching TV, to thinking for pleasure. To see whether this preference generalized outside of the United States, we replicated the study with 2,557 participants from 12 sites in 11 countries. The results were consistent in every country: Participants randomly assigned to do somethi...
6 CitationsSource
#1Shelly L. Gable (UCSB: University of California, Santa Barbara)H-Index: 49
#2Elizabeth A. Hopper (UCSB: University of California, Santa Barbara)H-Index: 2
Last. Jonathan W. Schooler (UCSB: University of California, Santa Barbara)H-Index: 78
view all 3 authors...
How often are creative ideas generated during episodes of mind wandering, and do they differ from those generated while on task? In two studies (N = 98, N = 87), professional writers and physicists...
19 CitationsSource
#1Dolores Albarracín (UIUC: University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign)H-Index: 50
#2Aashna Sunderrajan (UIUC: University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign)H-Index: 3
Last. Benjamin X. White (UIUC: University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign)H-Index: 2
view all 4 authors...
Abstract This chapter presents a model of the cognitive, motivational, and behavioral implications of the concepts of action and inaction. The action or inaction nature of a behavior is a judgment, a subjective and variable construal, made by the actor or an observer based on concepts of action and inaction. In other literatures, action and inaction concepts entail affirming or negating of any behavior, but we posit that they are best defined in terms of level of effort and intentionality. Peopl...
4 CitationsSource
#1Paul Seli (Harvard University)H-Index: 27
#2Michael J. Kane (UNCG: University of North Carolina at Greensboro)H-Index: 51
Last. Daniel Smilek (UW: University of Waterloo)H-Index: 57
view all 7 authors...
As empirical research on mind-wandering accelerates, we draw attention to an emerging trend in how mind-wandering is conceptualized. Previously articulated definitions of mind-wandering differ from each other in important ways, yet they also maintain overlapping characteristics. This conceptual structure suggests that mind-wandering is best considered from a family-resemblances perspective, which entails treating it as a graded, heterogeneous construct and clearly measuring and describing the sp...
116 CitationsSource
#1L. Parker Schiffer (CGU: Claremont Graduate University)H-Index: 1
#2Tomi-Ann Roberts (CC: Colorado College)H-Index: 23
AbstractAchieving and maintaining happiness can be difficult, especially when people are not engaging in the activities they know facilitate it. ‘Flow’ activities require clear rules, challenge, a high investment of energy, and have been shown to promote long-term happiness better than low investment, passive activities. Do people know this? In two studies, we found that, despite recognizing that flow activities are more likely to facilitate happiness, people nevertheless participated in passive...
5 CitationsSource
#1Pablo Briñol (UAM: Autonomous University of Madrid)H-Index: 46
#2Richard E. Petty (OSU: Ohio State University)H-Index: 143
Last. Darío Díaz (UCLM: University of Castilla–La Mancha)H-Index: 13
view all 6 authors...
Anger, disgust, surprise, and awe are multifaceted emotions. Both anger and disgust are associated with feeling unpleasant as well as experiencing a sense of confidence, whereas surprise and awe tend to be more pleasant emotions that are associated with doubt. Most prior work has examined how appraisals (confidence, pleasantness) lead people to experience different emotions or to experience different levels of intensity within the same emotion. Instead, the current research focused on the conseq...
21 CitationsSource
#1Hallgeir Sjåstad (NHH: Norwegian School of Economics)H-Index: 5
#2Roy F. Baumeister (FSU: Florida State University)H-Index: 173
Planning is a future-directed thought process that is highly beneficial, but it requires mental effort. Informed by the strength model of self-regulation, four studies (N = 546) tested the hypothesis that willingness to plan is linked to good self-control. A correlational study (N = 201) found that people high in trait self-control had done more planning than other people and also intended to make more plans during the upcoming week. A laboratory experiment (N = 105) induced a state of ego deple...
24 CitationsSource
#1Michael Inzlicht (U of T: University of Toronto)H-Index: 65
#2Amitai Shenhav (Brown University)H-Index: 19
Last. Christopher Y. Olivola (CMU: Carnegie Mellon University)H-Index: 20
view all 3 authors...
According to prominent models in cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and economics, effort (be it physical or mental) is costly: when given a choice, humans and non-human animals alike tend to avoid effort. Here, we suggest that the opposite is also true and review extensive evidence that effort can also add value. Not only can the same outcomes be more rewarding if we apply more (not less) effort, sometimes we select options precisely because they require effort. Given the increasing recognitio...
137 CitationsSource
AbstractAs humans, we have the ability to travel beyond the present moment by considering past and future times as well as locations that are distant from oneself. An interesting possibility is that such mental travel to far-away times and places (vs. focusing on times and places closer to the present) may increase people’s desire to search for their lives’ meaning. Since searching for meaning involves the integration of separate life events into a coherent life story, mentally traveling farther...
1 CitationsSource
#1Erin C. Westgate (UVA: University of Virginia)H-Index: 12
#2Timothy D. Wilson (UVA: University of Virginia)H-Index: 78
: What is boredom? We review environmental, attentional, and functional theories and present a new model that describes boredom as an affective indicator of unsuccessful attentional engagement in valued goal-congruent activity. According to the Meaning and Attentional Components (MAC) model, boredom is the result of (a) an attentional component, namely mismatches between cognitive demands and available mental resources, and (b) a meaning component, namely mismatches between activities and valued...
53 CitationsSource
Cited By2
Newest
#1Ana CancelaH-Index: 4
#2Pablo Briñol (UAM: Autonomous University of Madrid)H-Index: 46
Last. Richard E. Petty (OSU: Ohio State University)H-Index: 143
view all 3 authors...
Practitioners and researchers interested in designing wise interventions often recommend increasing personal involvement to be successful. Early research demonstrated that personal involvement increases elaboration leading to more persuasion for strong arguments, but to reduced persuasion if the arguments presented are specious. In most prior work, message recipients were plausibly motivated by their desire for knowledge. In the current research, we compare this epistemic goal to another goal in...
Source
#1Pablo Briñol (UAM: Autonomous University of Madrid)H-Index: 46
#2Richard E. Petty (OSU: Ohio State University)H-Index: 143
We review work from persuasion science relevant to reducing prejudiced attitudes. We begin by introducing the idea that the thoughts people generate – their number and valence – are critical for un...
Source
#1Pablo Briñol (UAM: Autonomous University of Madrid)H-Index: 46
#2Richard E. Petty (OSU: Ohio State University)H-Index: 143
Last. Lorena Ortega Moreno (UAM: Autonomous University of Madrid)H-Index: 5
view all 4 authors...
Can people improve their lives by smiling more, trying to have a better posture, and by thinking about good memories? Can individuals become more successful by deliberatively engaging in positive actions and thoughts? Do people feel better by following recommendations from naive psychology? In the present article we discuss these questions, noting that although some popular interventions thought to be universally beneficial (e.g., inductions of happiness, self-affirmation, empowerment, self-dist...
3 CitationsSource
#1Anseel FH-Index: 1
#2Madeline OngH-Index: 4
1 CitationsSource
#1David M. Markowitz (UO: University of Oregon)H-Index: 8
#2Jeffrey T. Hancock (Stanford University)H-Index: 53
Last. Byron Reeves (Stanford University)H-Index: 47
view all 4 authors...
This preregistered study examined the psychological and physiological consequences of exercising self-control with the mobile phone. A total of 125 participants were randomly assigned to sit in an unadorned room for six minutes and either (a) use their mobile phone, (b) sit alone with no phone, or (c) sit with their device but resist using it. Consistent with prior work, participants self-reported more concentration difficulty and more mind wandering with no device present compared to using the ...
3 CitationsSource